default_mobilelogo

ΓΛΩΣΣΑ - LANGUAGE

ΕΥΡΕΤΗΡΙΟ ΑΡΘΡΩΝ

Ιστότοπος Ποιότητας στην Υγεία

Τετάρτη, 16 Απριλίου 2014 03:00

Language in Profoundly Deaf People with Schizophrenia (Gerasimos Chatzidamianos)

Βαθμολογήστε αυτό το άρθρο
(1 Ψήφος)

Dr Gerasimos Chatzidamianos Ph.D. (Cantab), MPhil. (Cantab), BSc (Hons)

Spectrum Centre for Mental Health Research

Division of Health Research, Faculty of Health and Medicine

Lancaster University, Lancaster, U.K.,

 

Schizophrenia is known to occur in prelingually profoundly Deaf individuals with probably the same frequency as in the hearing population. However, the impact of Deafness on the clinical features of schizophrenia is an under-researched area. The current thesis aims to provide the first comprehensive analysis of the ways schizophrenia manifests itself in Deaf adults, with particular reference to language disorders.

Two studies were performed. Study 1 empirically tested a clinical observation that motor dexterity for sign was preserved despite there being impairment in motor skill for purposes other than language in schizophrenia. 15 profoundly Deaf sign-using schizophrenic patients and 28 matched profoundly Deaf healthy volunteers were given specially devised measures for motor skill in linguistic and non-linguistic tasks. The study supported the hypothesis that there is a dissociation between relatively preserved motor skill for sign language and impaired motor skill for non-linguistic gestural tasks. This study also produced an incidental observation: the Deaf schizophrenic patients appeared to make frequent errors in handshape, which in the context of the study implied abnormality in production of a particular linguistic element of sign language, classifiers.

The aim in Study 2 was to further examine Study 1’s finding of abnormality in the production of handshapes in classifier construction, specifically to replicate it under controlled conditions and establish whether it is more pronounced than in other aspects of language. The performance of a second group of 14 profoundly Deaf signing schizophrenic patients and 35 Deaf healthy volunteers was recorded based on a battery of measures testing classifier and noun comprehension and production. This confirmed Study 1’s finding of errors in classifier production in Deaf schizophrenic patients and provided qualified support for the hypothesis that the impairment was more marked for production than comprehension, and more marked for classifiers than for nouns.

Taking the results of the analyses of both studies together, the present thesis suggests that motor abnormalities are present in Deaf schizophrenia in the face of relatively intact motor skill for language. Despite its motor intactness, however, certain aspects of sign language in Deaf schizophrenia seem to be disproportionately affected than others (e.g. nouns). This primarily applies to the production of handshape in classifier construction. Conversely, Deaf people with schizophrenia appear to produce lexicalised responses at a comparable rate with that of their Deaf healthy counterparts. These results suggest that schizophrenia affects language production in Deaf patients with schizophrenia in unique ways.

Clinical implications

Being an under-research area, understanding how differently schizophrenia manifests itself in the Deaf population is critical. Whilst the prevalence of schizophrenia in the Deaf is similar to that of the hearing population, Deaf people are over-represented in psychiatric services. The underlying reasons are complicated, but it becomes apparent that the varied expression of schizophrenia, coupled with the idiosyncratic language proficiency as found in Deaf people increases the level of difficulty in diagnosing the condition. This is particularly relevant in the case of Deaf people as most clinician are unfamiliar with Deafness, sign language and the implication of growing up in a hearing world without access to appropriate education, information or services.

The present results put forward the idea that clinicians should expect language production to be motorically fine. If severe motor symptoms present, clinicians should investigate possible extrapyramidal side effects from the medication or the potential comorbidity with other neurological disorders. Despite its motor intactness, however, sign language production appears disproportionately impaired when compared to sign comprehension. That means that Deaf people with schizophrenia are able to understand sign sufficiently, but fail to produce it to a relatively similar degree. This is not to say that their linguistic output is incomprehensible similar to dysphasia, for example. In effect, Deaf people with schizophrenia should have no problem understand their clinicians in sign language. The marked but subtle impairment in production (especially in classifier constructions), however, increases the challenges in identifying symptoms such as formal thought disorder (i.e. incoherence of speech/sign). Given that lexicalisation of sign appears intact, clinicians should be mindful of these challenges and adjust their signing accordingly.

 

Bibliography

Altshuler, K.Z. (1971). Studies of the deaf: relevance to psychiatric theory
American Journal of Psychiatry127, 1521-1526.

Atkinson, J.R., Gleeson, K., Cromwell, J., & O’Rourke, S.(2007)Exploring the Perceptual Characteristics of Voice-Hallucinations in Deaf People.Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12(4), 339-361.

Basilier, T. (1964). Surdophrenia: the Psychic Consequences of Congenital or Early Acquired Deafness. Acta Psychiatrica Scadinavica40, 362-372.

Basilier, T. (1973). Hørselstap og egentlig døvhet i socialpsykiatrisk perspektiv [trans. Hearing loss and genuine deafness in a social psychiatric perspective]. Oslo, Norway: Universitetsforlaget.

Black, P., & Glickman, N.S. (2006). Demographics, Psychiatric Diagnoses, and Other Characteristics of North American Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Inpatients. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education11(3), 303-321.

Briffa, D. (1999a). Hallucinations in Deaf People with mental illness: lessons from the Deaf Client. Australian Psychiatry7(2), 72-74.

Briffa, D. (1999b). Deaf and Mentally ill: Are their needs being met? Australian Psychiatry7(1), 7-10.

Chaika, E.O. (1974). A linguist looks at “schizophrenic” language. Brain Language1, 257- 276.

Cooper, A.F., Curry, A.R., Kay, D.W., Garside, R.F., & Roth, M., (1974). Hearing loss in paranoid and affective psychoses of the elderly. Lancet2, 851-854.

Critchley, E., Denmark, J., Warren, F., & Wilson, K. (1981). Hallucinatory Experience of Prelingually Profoundly Deaf Schizophrenics. British Journal of Psychiatry138, 30-32.

Denmark, J.C. (1966). Mental illness and early profound deafness. British Journal of Psychology39, 117-124.

Denmark, J.C. (1994). Deafness and Mental Health. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers Limited.

Denmark, J.C., & Eldridge, R.W. (1969).Psychiatric Services for the Deaf.The Lancet2, 259-262.

Denmark, J.C., & Warren, F. (1972). A Psychiatric Unit for the DeafThe British Journal of Psychiatry120, 423-428.

du Feu, M., & Chovaz, C. (2014). Mental health and deafness. NW: Oxford University Press.

du Feu, M., & Fergusson, K. (2003). Sensory impairment and mental health. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment9, 95-103.

du Feu, M., & McKenna, P.J. (1999). Auditory hallucinations in profoundly deaf schizophrenic patients: a phenomenological analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica99(6), 453-459.

Emmorey, K. (2003). Perspectives on Classifier Constructions in Sign Languages. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Hindley, P. (1993). Signs of Feeling. A prevalence study of psychiatric disorder in deaf and partially hearing children and adolescents. RNID, London.

Hindley, P., & Kitson, N. (2000). Mental Health and Deafness. London: Whurr.

Kitson, N., & Fry, R. (1990). Prelingual Deafness and Psychiatry. British Journal of Hospital Medicine44, 353-356.

McKenna, P.J. (2007). Schizophrenia and Related Syndromes (2nd Eds.). U.K.: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.

McKenna, P.J., & Oh, T.M. (2005). Schizophrenic Speech: Making Sense of Bathroots and Ponds that Fall in Doorways. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Poizner, H., Klima, E.S., Bellugi, U. (1987). What the hands reveal about the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Remvig, J. (1969). Three clinical studies of Deaf-Mutism and Psychiatry. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 36 (Suppl. 210), 1-120.

SchonauerK., Achtergarde, D., Suslow, T., Michael, N. (1999). Comorbidity of schizophrenia and prelingual deafness: its impact on social network structures.Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology,34526-532.

Steinberg, A.G., Sullivan, V.J., Loew, R.C. (1998). Cultural and Linguistic Barriers to Mental Health Service Access: The Deaf Consumer’s Perspective. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155(7), 982-984.

Supalla, T. (1986). The classifier system in American Sign Language. In C. Craig (Eds.) Noun classification and categorization, Philadelphia: Benjamin, pp.181-214.

Thacker, A. (1994). Formal thought disorder. Sign language in deaf people with schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165(6), 818-23.

Trumbetta, S.L., Bonvillian, J.D., Siedlecki, T., Haskins, B.G. (2001). Language-related symptoms in persons with schizophrenia and how deaf persons may manifest these symptoms. Sign Language Studies, 1(3), 228-253.

Vernon, M. (2005). Fifty Years of Research on the Intelligence of Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Children: A Review of Literature and Discussion of Implications. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education10(3), 225-231.

Vernon, M., & Daigle-King, B. (1999). Historical overview of inpatient care of mental patients who are deaf. American Annals of the Deaf, 144, 51-61.

Διαβάστηκε 6576 φορές

Website Security Test

ΟΡΟΙ ΧΡΗΣΗΣ